Chronic Stress, Cortical Plasticity and Neuroecology
Abstract
Prolonged psychological stress and accompanying elevations
in blood cortisol are known to induce hypometabolism and decreasing synaptic
density in the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This article
evaluates and explores evidence supporting the hypothesis that these, and
other, selective effects of prolonged stress constitute a neuroecological
program that adaptively modifies behavior in mammals experiencing adverse
conditions. Three complementary hypotheses are proposed: 1) chronic stress
signifies that the prevailing environment is life-threatening, indicating that
the animal should decrease activity in brain areas capable of inhibiting the
stress axis; 2) stress signifies that the environment is unpredictable, that
high-level cognition may be less effective, and that the animal should increase
its reliance on defensive, procedural and instinctual behaviors mediated by
lower brain centers; and 3) stress indicates that environmental events are
proving difficult to systemize based on delayed associations, and thus the
maintenance of contextual, task-relevant information in the PFC need not be
maintained for temporally-extended periods. Humans, along with countless other
species of vertebrates, have been shown to make predictive, adaptive responses
to chronic stress in many systems including metabolic, cardiovascular,
neuroendocrine, and even amygdalar and striatal systems. It is proposed in this
article that humans and other mammals may also have an inducible,
cerebrocortical response to pronounced stress that mediates a transition from
time-intensive, explicit (controlled/attentional/top-down) processing of
information to quick, implicit (automatic/preattentive/bottom-up) processing.
Keywords:
cortisol, evolution, executive control, hippocampus, neuroecology, prefrontal
cortex, stress cascade, top-down processing
1.0 Chronic Stress,
Cortical Plasticity and Neuroecology
Organisms throughout the five kingdoms retain certain
capacities to adaptively modify their phenotype in order to better conform to
their environment (Auld et al., 2010). Some of these changes are transient and
reversible, whereas some are comprehensive and permanent. The studies of
phenotypic plasticity, polyphenism and “predictive, adaptive responses” have
shown that virtually all species can be reprogrammed by portending
environmental cues, that the morphological changes are brought about by
alterations in gene expression, and that the changes allow conformation to
occasional but regularly recurring environmental pressures (DeWitt &
Scheiner, 2004). These alternate environments typically involve stressors which
demand different body types, behaviors, reproductive tactics, and life-history
strategies (Pigliucci, 2001). Often the adaptive response to stress is
conserved within groups of closely related organisms that inhabit similar
ecological niches (Via & Lande, 1985). For instance, even though all
organisms respond plastically to nutrient/energy deprivation, mammals exhibit a
unique suite of physiological changes aimed at lowering the metabolism of
specific organ systems in the interest of continued survival (Wells, 2009).
This article discusses phenotypic changes in mammalian brain structure and
neurochemistry, known to be largely mediated by alterations in gene expression,
that occur in response to chronically high levels of the stress hormone
cortisol. Herein, well-documented brain changes, and their behavioral
correlates, are characterized as potentially adaptive responses to adverse
ecological scenarios. Different lines of converging evidence will be considered
in an exploratory and expository manner.
The mature mammalian brain can be reshaped by chronic or
prolonged stress in two primary ways: 1) metabolic activity, dendritic growth
and implicit memory are enhanced in the amygdala and caudate nucleus; and 2)
metabolic activity, dendritic growth, explicit memory and inhibitory functions
are reduced in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Cohen et al., 2007;
Sapolsky, 2003). Many of the effects of stress on neural circuitry are mediated
by the stress hormone cortisol which activates the numerous cortisol receptors
present in the amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC (Morales-Medina et al., 2009).
Once activated, these receptors trigger pathways that result in the expression
or silencing of particular genes, which are the molecular antecedents thought
to be responsible for a large proportion of the neurological remodeling
(Petronis, 2000, 2004; DeWitt & Scheiner, 2004). This remodeling, much of
which has been shown to be epigenetic, may help stressed mammals to adapt to
environmental adversity, with its particular set of recurrent and ecologically
relevant threats and opportunities.
In the literature, the responses to stress in the amygdala
and basal ganglia have been attributed adaptive significance (Sapolsky, 2003),
but the responses of the hippocampus and PFC have mostly eluded the attention
of evolutionary biologists (Reser, 2007). Increased activity in the amygdala is
thought to help animals become more sensitive and responsive to threat (Radley
& Morrison, 2005). Neural and dendritic hypertrophy in the basolateral
amygdala potentiates the mechanisms dedicated to identifying stressors, and
mobilizing the body to address them (Sapolsky, 2003). The amygdala stimulates
the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) to release stress
hormones, and hypertrophy of the amygdala increases its capacity to do this
(Roozendaal et al., 2009). A different way to potentiate activity in the
amygdala is to release it from the structures that tonically inhibit it (Mitra
& Sapolsky, 2008). The PFC and hippocampus have long been identified in
neurology as brain regions capable of inhibiting the autonomic and emotional
responses to fear-inducing stimuli (Cannon, 1929; Papez, 1937; MacLean, 1949;
see LeDoux, 1987, for a review). This circuitry ensures that mammals can
override the fear response if they make the determination that the stimulus may
appear threatening but is not actually threatening (Morgan et al., 1993).
Diminishment of activity in the PFC and hippocampus may ensure that the areas
that incite stress, the amygdala and PVN, can function unimpeded during
stressful times.
Decreased activity in the PFC and hippocampus may also
adaptively influence the animal to be less cerebral and more impulsive (Reser,
2007). When facing lasting adversity, it may be advantageous to suppress the
PFC and hippocampus because these areas put inhibitory pressure on defensive,
instinctual, and dominant responses. When an animal experiences extreme stress,
it is probable that its high-order behavioral strategies are proving relatively
ineffectual (Boonstra, 2005). It may benefit such an animal to be less reliant
on learned behavior, and more reliant on genetically programmed and
species-specific behaviors. Hence, the changes in the hippocampus and PFC may
protectively disinhibit innate and instinctual urges (Reser, 2007).
The present article will elaborate on three complementary
hypotheses: 1) stress signifies that the prevailing environment is antagonistic,
and that the animal should not suppress the stress response or inhibit
conditioned fears; 2) stress signifies that behaviors that the animal has
learned may be inefficacious or deleterious and that it should increase its
reliance on innate behaviors over learned behaviors; and 3) stress indicates
that environmental events are proving difficult to systemize on long time
scales (using delayed associations) and thus the maintenance of contextual,
task-relevant information in the PFC need not be maintained for
temporally-extended periods.
Several neurological changes to areas including the
amygdala, the caudate nucleus, the hippocampus, the mPFC, and the PFC in
general will be discussed. Table 1 describes the general psychological
consequences of these changes, the implications that they have for modern
people as well as hypothetical implications that they may have had for
prehistoric foragers. This table attempts to highlight the disparity between
the limiting repercussions of these changes in the modern “information age” and
their potentially adaptive significance in the prehistoric past.
Table 1
The Neurological
Effects of Stress, Then and Now
Neurological State
|
Psychological Consequences
|
Implications for Moderns
|
Implications for Foragers
|
Amygdala hyperactivity
|
Potentiation of conditioned
fears
|
Anxiety, fear and excessive stress
|
Healthy caution, preparedness and mobilization
|
Caudate hyperactivity
|
Potentiation of procedural
or habitual movements
|
Intrusion of habitual or
procedural responses
|
Increased reliance on
movements that have been proven effective
|
PFC hypoactivity
|
Behavioral disinhibition
|
Working memory and goal-setting problems
|
Increased reliance on instinctual and appetitive impulses
|
mPFC hypoactivity
|
Impaired inhibition of conditioned fears
|
Exaggerated stress
responses to nonfatal threats
|
Enhanced awareness of
potential threats
|
Hippocampal hypoactivity
|
Inaccessibility of contextual and episodic information
|
Explicit/ declarative memory problems
|
Increased reliance on dominant and procedural responses
|
Interestingly, prenatal and early-life stress cause a
pattern of changes that is strikingly similar to the changes that occur in
response to chronic stress in adulthood (Weinstock, 2008). When pregnant rodent
or primate mothers are stressed, they program highly analogous changes in the
amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC of their offspring (Francis et. al., 1999;
Kapoor et al., 2006; Schneider et al., 1999). The behavioral changes in these
offspring, which include increased vigilance, fearfulness and stress
responsivity, have been interpreted by Michael Meaney and colleagues as
constituting a predictive and adaptive response to early environmental
adversity (Zhang et al., 2006). In this interpretation the amygdalar changes
are attributed adaptive qualities. However, the role of the hippocampus and the
PFC in contributing to this behavioral response has been neglected. Moreover,
psychiatric disorders such as anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress
disorder and schizophrenia are associated with prenatal and postnatal stress,
and involve the same pattern of changes to the hippocampus, PFC and amygdala
(Axelson, 1993; Corcoran, 2001).
Elevated levels of noradrenaline and dopamine, such as occur
during acute yet transient stress, impair PFC and hippocampus-dependent
abilities such as working memory and attention regulation, but strengthen
amygdala, caudate and subcortical-dependent functions such as fear
conditioning, habitual behaviors and reflexes (Elliot & Packard, 2008;
Packard & Teather, 1998). Thus, acute stress, chronic stress, prenatal
stress and a number of major psychiatric disorders have all been shown to
engineer a switch from thoughtful “top-down” control based on task-relevance to
bottom-up control based on salience (Buschman & Miller, 2007; Hermans et
al., 2014). This article focuses on these cortical corollaries of pronounced
stress, and attempts to interpret them in terms of their ecological utility to
mammals, from wild rodents to prehistoric humans. If the neurological changes
that respond to stress were diffuse or only degenerative this might indicate
that they do not represent adaptation. That the alterations are very selective,
that they completely spare critical cortical and subcortical regions, that
there are dozens of documented molecular pathways that converge toward these
changes (Stankiewicz et al., 2013), and that arborization and neural activity
in the amygdala (Francis et al., 1999; Radley & Morrison, 2005; Vyas et
al., 2003) and caudate (Kim et al., 2001; Schwabe et al., 2008) is actually
enhanced, suggests that these changes may not be pathological. To further
explore the proposed evolutionary rationale for why these changes, in adulthood
and in utero, might constitute an adaptive response we turn to the neurobiology
of stress perception.
The rest of the article can be found here:
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635716301334
Here are some passages from an earlier version of the manuscript that I removed:
It will be interesting to see how reflexes are potentiated
by stress aside from hyperactivity in the caudate nucleus. I imagine that it is possible that the corticospinal system (which is
controlled by the frontal cortex) is downregulated relative to the
extrapyramidal motor systems. It is possible that the primary motor cortex
(which commands the cortiospinal (pyramidal) system is unchanged but the areas
that regulate and inhibit its activity (like the premotor, supplementary motor
and presupplementary motor areas) are downregulated. In fact, the
presupplementary motor cortex but not the primary motor cortex is damaged in schizophrenia. This means that
stress makes you worse at planning complex sequences of movements.
The stress cascade acts to reduce the amount of information
that individuals take with them through time. The PFC generally learns to pick
out key stimuli from different scenarios
The more stressed an animal has been the fewer elements of its immediate
past can be recalled attentively and applied to the current situation. Animal
species that have been endowed with large working memories by evolution can
expect that it will be helpful to have many brain areas, corresponding to the
last several seconds of thought (or experience), primed at once so that they
may bring elements of their immediate past to bear on the present. Less
intelligent animals can take only the most salient aspect with them through
time, and old, hardwired or subcortical areas are what programs most of their
actions in the present.
Here are some passages from an earlier version of the manuscript that I removed:
The PFC and hippocampus are probably targeted by the stress
cascade because of their subtle contributions to behavior. They are both
multimodal convergence areas that integrate information from multiple sensory
areas to allow sensory reconciliation and ultimately self-control and
self-guided action. These two areas do not control any basic unitary ability
though. Most other areas of cortex, if rendered hypometabolic, would result in
the loss of motor skills, or sensory processing. For instance, pronounced
dendritic shrinkage in the occipital lobe would adversely affect or even
abolish certain visual abilities. Yet the neuropathology in the stress cascade
spares early sensory areas and even most association areas and is confined to
the more expendable, more abstract areas...
Cortical dendritic spines are highly motile structures that
are capable of responding quickly to physiological and environmental input -
responses that are integral to learning and behavioral plasticity (Kirov et
al., 2004). Restricting their growth and actually retracting them should limit
behavioral plasticity and also reduce the accessibility of information
(memories) that is encoded by the synaptic weights. That the apical, but not
the basal dendrites are affected (Brown et al., 2005) suggests that this
response is designed to reduce the contribution of higher cortical layers. The
superficial layers, II and III, are responsible for sending and receiving
recurrent (feedback/ topdown/ modulatory) signals that encode subtle distinctions
of learned information (Calvin, 1995). This epigenetic program spares the lower
layers which receive inputs from and produce outputs to subcortical areas. Such
apical degeneration could make it so that cortical areas are accessible to
lower areas, only they are being used to a lesser extent. In other words,
during stress it is still important to draw vertically from various cortical
columns, or clusters of cells with similar receptive fields, however; it is
less important to draw from the higher cortical lamina (layers II and III).
This makes the content of memories accessible, but minimizes the horizontal
cross-talk within higher layers, perhaps resulting in simplified answers to
behavioral questions. It emphasizes bottom up at the expense of top down
processing. Cognition without the horizontal information-sharing decreases the
temporal persistence of information processing, potentially contributing to the
documented diminishments in working memory and inhibition. It should be very
interesting to better resolve how the debranching of apical dendrites affects
the processing within and between individual cortical columns...
The dendritic changes in the medial PFC follow another
definite pattern where larger spines are affected to a greater extent. Larger
dendritic spines which are less plastic, have larger synapses, a high
proportion of AMPA receptors, are stable, and are thought to represent “memory
spines” (Kasai et al., 2003). Small spines are more plastic, labile, have
smaller synapses with a high proportion of NMDA receptors, and can either
retract or become larger and stable and these are thought to represent
“learning spines.” During stress the larger spines are the ones that retract
the most (Cerqueira et al., 2007), hence it should be interesting to determine
how this affects the cell’s receptive field, and why. Clearly there is a
biological favoritism here. It should be important to understand why larger
spines should retract. This seems to be consistent with the second hypothesis
described in the introduction, suggesting that stress signifies that learned
behavior may be ineffectual, and that the animal should increase its reliance
on instinctual and procedural behaviors...
Perhaps the stress cascade makes an
animal hesitant to stray far from home to keep it safe. And perhaps it also
makes it so that the animal’s inclination to go out and explore, to be an
infovore and search out new areas, because it has mastered the old ones, is
diminished.
Many animals, such as a large proportion of invertebrates,
that inhabit simple ecological niches have very simple nervous systems and come
into the world with nearly all of the behavior needed preprogrammed into their
bodies by their genes. These animals can only learn very rudimentary things
that only slightly modify their innate systems. As brain to body ratio and encephalization have increased in
the animal kingdom, over the last 700 million years, many species have been
granted increasing autonomy over their own behavior. More intelligent animals, like those in
our phylum, chordates, have the ability to be programmed by the environment so
that the behavior of any two individuals of the same species may be very
different especially if they developed in differing environments.
The mammalian cortex,
which is highly developed in humans, allows very high degrees of behavioral
plasticity for planned actions (Moscovich, 2007). Such a high degree of
self-government and freedom from instinct would potentially be very dangerous
for animals such as reptiles, amphibeans and fish because they would probably
often be hard pressed to develop adaptive behaviors on their own. A vastly
increased ability for new learning probably developed in mammals because of the
presence of maternal care and instruction. Developing mammals were unique
because they had doting models from whom to learn memes. But what if a young
mammal’s learning does not lead to adaptive behavior and the mother’s own
stress detracts from her ability to share knowledge? Might it then be
beneficial to return to instinctive behavior? If natural selection could have
accomplished it, it probably would have given mammals the connections needed at
birth to live, eat and procreate as it does in very simple invertebrates.
Mammalian ecological niches are so variable though, that genes could never
prepare them properly by preprogramming all of their behavior. For this reason
higher cognitive areas are a necessity although perhaps a calculated risk.
People cite many reasons for what pressures caused the human
brain to evolve: complex foraging techniques, hunting, social abilities,
Machiavellian intelligence. I think that a major contributor to the evolution
of human intelligence was the stress free period that resulted from the manufacture
of weapons. Intelligence evolved because hominids in our direct line of descent
(that we descended from lower forms is ironic) evolved to hunt in groups and
fashion weapons, this increased order and decreased predation (intraspecies
aggression probably stayed the same after weapons). Now that the world was
safer, people had time to see order. The abilities to think things all the way
through and to plan ahead were selected. The ability for self discipline and to
exercise control over wants was selected for. The ability to do the harder
thing and to hold out or to plan, to set
things up in advance and “wait for it to be a sure thing.” Sometimes newly
appreciated phenomena give us a new vantage point to see other things. I think
that the stress cascade gives us valuable insight into the evolution of human
intelligence and tells us that safety and order, allowed us time to deliberate
which allowed selection to act on the other cognitive theories like hunting,
social abilities and such. The way that you think about the things in your
environment determines what you extract from them. It seems likely that, like
with most other traits, there might be crucial genetic variations that ended up
causing an aneurotypical way to think about or perceive the environment. ADHD,
depression, PTSD and anxiety may all represent different but similarly adaptive
ways to extract specific information from a presumed adverse environment. The
predictive adaptive response of the stress cascade made the dangerous expansion
of the human mind, not so dangerous.
Perhaps only a minority of people are neurologically well
suited for occupations that demand cognitive rigor, planning, foresight and
intense concentration. Since there was no electrical engineering, computer
programming or rocket science in the ancestral past, sometimes it is unclear
how these skills were selected for during human evolution. Perhaps neurological
dispositions like these represent an optimistic outlook that expects a
systemizable and orderly environment. Delayed gratification, marshmallow
studies, waiting longer means more intelligence and school performance. Tie
between adversity and delay. Only in a good environment can you delay. The
brain evolved to learn about the things that we are motivated about. Today we
learn about things that have nothing to do with survival. Hominids probably
would have found it hard to get motivated about school work and occupational
expertise but would have stayed interested in activities that lead directly to
food, shelter and social interaction. And this would have been good for them.
The discipline of behavioral ecology is based on the premise
that cognitive traits and the neural substrates responsible for them are shaped
by natural selection and hence are fine tuned to respond to the traditional
environment of the species in question. A subdivision of this discipline,
called neuroecology, attempts to draw causal links from variations in brain
structure between species to the lifestyle of these species. For instance, a
species that forages over wide terrain or hides (caches) food more than a
similar species would be expected to have a larger hippocampus because the
hippocampus, an area responsible for spatial cognition, plays a bigger role in
its ecological behavior. Neuroecology has received some criticism because the
predictions that it tests are largely adaptationist (i.e. if it exists it must
have been naturally selected) but since has been largely redefined.
Reality testing of past memories is difficult for one main
reason, because the semantic aspects of a memory can be completely isolated
from the contextual. The way that we test a memory, to see if it was imagined
or not, is to try to recollect the context. I have a strong memory that my
friend misheard a song lyric in a popular song. The friend heard “she is a
Swedish drum” and the lyric is “she is the sweetest drug.” I asked my friend
about this over a year later and my friend denied it completely. I knew that it
took place and I scoured my memory… and got no where. All I could remember was
that the event took place, but I had no contextual cues. I can’t remember where
we were, what else we were saying, what time of day it was, or if I was alone
daydreaming the scenario to life. The only other thing I could remember,
besides the literal lyrics, was that it was a real memory, is this really
trustworthy though? Could the “this was real” memory actually just be a mutated
“wouldn’t it be funny if this had been real” memory? Maybe if it all comes back really FAST, then this indicates
that it is hippocampal and thus veridical in origin. If it comes back more
slowly, it could be confabulated or reconstructed.